RISS 학술연구정보서비스

검색
다국어 입력

http://chineseinput.net/에서 pinyin(병음)방식으로 중국어를 변환할 수 있습니다.

변환된 중국어를 복사하여 사용하시면 됩니다.

예시)
  • 中文 을 입력하시려면 zhongwen을 입력하시고 space를누르시면됩니다.
  • 北京 을 입력하시려면 beijing을 입력하시고 space를 누르시면 됩니다.
닫기
    인기검색어 순위 펼치기

    RISS 인기검색어

      검색결과 좁혀 보기

      선택해제
      • 좁혀본 항목 보기순서

        • 원문유무
        • 원문제공처
          펼치기
        • 등재정보
          펼치기
        • 학술지명
          펼치기
        • 주제분류
          펼치기
        • 발행연도
          펼치기
        • 작성언어

      오늘 본 자료

      • 오늘 본 자료가 없습니다.
      더보기
      • 무료
      • 기관 내 무료
      • 유료
      • KCI등재

        한국어 통사론의 나아갈 방향: 논항 구조에서 인칭 구조로의 전환

        목정수 한글학회 2019 한글 Vol.- No.326

        A comprehensive Korean syntax must begin by establishing basic syntactic units. The grammatical statuses of josa (조사) and eomi (어미) are so problematic that it is difficult to generate a consensus. Korean grammarians are divided into three groups with regard to this issue. The first group holds the synthetic view, that josa and eomi are not syntactic units. The second group holds the analytic view, that they are syntactic units. The third group holds the eclectic view, that josa is a syntactic unit but eomi is not. This study was conducted from the analytic view, which holds that “학교-에서-는” and “먹-었-겠-더-라” are syntactic constructions, not morphological constructions, and are composed of syntactic units in the following orders: noun (lexical element), postposition (grammatical element), and determiner (grammatical element); and verb (lexical element), prefinal ending (grammatical element), ending (grammatical element), respectively. This study proposed a dynamic tree diagram which shows the realities of the nominal expansion structure of josa and verbal expansion structure of eomi. This study concludes that the person structure should be used in addition to the argument structure because the person structure’s information is derived from endings. Using both structures allows for the complete analysis of Korean sentences.

      • KCI등재후보

        从“NP+的+VP”结构的外部功能看其内部构造

        이은경 한국중국언어문화연구회 2011 한중언어문화연구 Vol.- No.27

        This study focused on the syntactic function of the “NP+de+VP” structure (syntactic distribution), to examine when we put this dependent structure into a larger syntactic structure, what kind of syntactic position that can take, what kind of syntactic and semantic constrains exist with its related components. Then, from the structure of the external features and characteristics come back to see the internal construction and the originate of the structure. In other words, through this structure’s external features and limited options, test the two viewpiont about that construction of its structure and sources ; (1) the predicate component “VP” of the “NP+de+VP” is first nomialized, then by structural modification “de”, constitute the nominal-modified structure. This theoretical assumption can be called “The partial nominalization viewpoint”. (2)“NP+de+VP” is a nominalization form by inserting the nominalizational marker de in the subject-predicate construction, rather than an endocentric construction in which the predicate component “VP” not nominalized. This theoretical assumption can be called “The whole nominalization viewpoint”. Finally, this study found the “The whole nominalization viewpoint” of the structure is more logically consistent, more in line with the language fact than “The partial nominalization viewpoint”.

      • KCI등재

        일본어 イ형 보조동사문의 통사구조에 대한 의미해석 방법

        박용일(Park, Yong Il) 동아시아일본학회 2014 일본문화연구 Vol.0 No.52

        This paper is about the syntactic structure and interpretation of [i-] type auxiliary verb sentence in Japanese, especially, focused on the analysis method of the correlation between syntactic structure and interpretation. According to precedent studies, Japanese auxiliary-verb sentences are separated into two types. One is [i-] type, the other is [te-] type. In this paper, we have explained the structure of [i-] type sentences, first. All [i-] type sentences have a bi-clausal structure in the initial structure. However, in the surface structure, some [i-] type sentences have only a mono-clausal structure which is correlated with one interpretation, and some [i-] type sentences have a mono-structure and a bi-structure which are correlated with two interpretations. The above results on the correlation of structure and interpretation are proved by the scope phenomenon of the [dake-nai]. In other words, this study shows the fact that the scope phenomenon of [dake-nai] is valid for the analysis method of correlation between interpretation and syntactic structure in Japanese.

      • KCI등재

        복문으로 나타나는 일본어 「知る」문과 한국어 「알다」문의 통사구조와 재구조화현상

        박용일 한양대학교 일본학국제비교연구소 2021 비교일본학 Vol.51 No.-

        본 연구는 복문으로 나타나는 일본어의 [知る]문과 이와 대응하는 한국어 [알다]문의 심층구조, 기능적 구조 그리고 표층구조의 특징으로 단문화현상을 고찰하였다. 구체적으로는 일본어의 [知る]문과 이와 대응하여 나타나는 한국어 [알다]문은 술어의 의미역으로 인해 심층구조에서 복문구조로 생성되지만 이들 구문들이 어떤 통사적 기능을 보이냐에 따라서 컨트롤구조와 상승구조로 구분할 수 있음을 명시하였다. 즉, [こと] 보문소를 취하는 일본어의 [知る]문과 [일] 보문소를 취하는 한국어 [알다]문은 상승구조이며, [の] 보문소를 취하는 [知る]문과 [것] 보문소를 취하는 [알다]문은 컨트롤구조임을 명시하였다. 이와 같은 통사구조를 제시하기 위하여 본 고찰에서는 관용표현이 이들 구문에서 어떠한 해석으로 나타나는지에 대한 의미해석을 이용하였다. 또한 일본어의 [知る]문과 한국어 [알다]문의 표층구조에서는 모두 단문구조가 갖는 통사현상이 나타난다는 사실을 분열문현상을 통해 제시함으로써 이들 구문에서 재구조화현상이 관찰된다는 사실을 명시하였다. This study examines the syntactic structures and restructuring phenomena of the “siru” sentences in Japanese and the corresponding “alda” sentences in Korean that appear in complex sentences. More specifically, the study shows that although the “siru” sentences in Japanese and the corresponding “alda” sentences in Korean are formed as complex sentences in the deep structure due to the semantic role of the predicates, such syntactic structures can be divided into control structures or raising structures depending on the syntactic roles they perform. In a nutshell, the “siru” sentences in Japanese that take the complementizer of “koto” and the “alda” sentences in Korean that take the complementizer of “il” are of an raising structure, whereas the “siru” sentences that take the complementizer of “no” and “alda” sentences that take the complementizer of “geot” are of a control structure. To present such syntactic structures, this study focuses on the semantic phenomenon related to how idiomatic expressions are interpreted in such syntactic structures. In addition, this study shows that restructuring is observed in these syntactic structures, by demonstrating, through the incidences of cleft sentences, the fact that both the “siru” sentence in Japanese and the “alda” sentence in Korean show in their surface structure such syntactic phenomena as are found in simple-sentence structures.

      • KCI등재

        ≪论衡≫“V得~”结构的特征及其语法化进度研究

        谢卫菊 동아인문학회 2025 동아인문학 Vol.70 No.-

        ≪论衡≫“V得~”结构具有鲜明的过渡性时代特征。语义上,交代行为的“V”与补充说明结果的“得”之间存在“行为-结果”语义关系;句法上,行为“V”在前,结果“得”在后;语用上,动态语境中,行为“V”与结果“得”之间形成了“结构中心-表达重心”式语用结构。这些句法、语义和语用特征与补语语法化的要求一致,所以≪论衡≫中的“V得~”结构终将朝着述补宾结构的方向发展演变,但各种插入成分的介入阻碍了“V得~”结构的语法化进度。≪论衡≫中,已经实现了语法化的述补宾结构“V得~”、尚未完成语法化的连谓结构“V得~”,以及连谓结构的前身——叙述型结构“V,得”三者共存,这一语法现象体现了“V得~”结构的过渡性时代特征。各结构间的句法、语义和语用关联性又促成了各自之间的发展演变关系。 The “V得~” structure in Lunheng exhibits distinct transitional characteristics of its era. Semantically, there exists a “behavior-result” semantic relationship between the action denoted by “V” and the result expressed by “得.” Syntactically, the action “V” precedes, while the result “得” follows. Pragmatically, in a dynamic context, a “structural center- expressive focus” pragmatic structure is formed between the action “V” and the result “得.” These syntactic, semantic, and pragmatic features align with the requirements of Complement grammaticalization. Therefore, the “V得~” structure in Lunheng is ultimately evolving toward a Verb- Complement-Object structure. However, the intervention of various inserted elements has hindered the grammaticalization process of the “V得~” structure. In Lunheng, three structures coexist: the Verb-Complement-Object structure “V得~” which has undergone grammaticalization; the Serial Verb structure “V得~” which has not yet completed grammaticalization; and the Narrative structure “V, 得” which serves as the predecessor of the Serial Verb structure. This grammatical phenomenon reflects the transitional characteristics of the “V得~” structure. The syntactic, semantic, and pragmatic correlations among these structures have further facilitated their respective developmental and evolutionary relationships.

      • KCI등재

        보조용언(補助用言)의 판별(判別) 기준(基準)과 통사구조(統辭構造)의 상관관계(相關關係)에 대한 연구(硏究)

        조경순(曺炅舜) ( Jo¸ Kyung-sun ) 한국어문교육연구회 2021 어문연구 Vol.49 No.3

        Auxiliary verbs are defined relative to main verbs, and the criteria for distinguishing them from compound verbs and continuous verbs have been established. There is a problem that some criteria are identical to characteristics and the phenomenon caused by the syntactic structure of auxiliary verbs is included in the criteria. The characteristics of internal non-separability, separability substitution, inversion impossibility, and constraints on the position of the first and last endings are the phenomena of syntactic structure according to the syntactic bonding layer of auxiliary verbs. In this paper, the syntactic structure was analyzed and determined according to the number of essential arguments in the auxiliary verb syntax. Since there are sentences in which the auxiliary verb requires an argument, the auxiliary verb is combined after the main verb is combined with its ending. Internal inseparability is due to the combination of the suffix V′, which is the nucleus of CP, and the substitution and inversion of the main and auxiliary verbs are due to their hierarchical bonding structure. Through the syntactic structure of auxiliary verbs, it can be seen that the criterion for discrimination of auxiliary verbs is not a unitary phenomenon, but reflects syntactic phenomena arising from the syntactic combinations of auxiliary verbs.

      • KCI등재

        時量補語的句法及語用分析— 以VCO结构和VOVC结构为中心 —

        곡효여 한국중국어교육학회 2019 중국어교육과연구 Vol.0 No.29

        This paper analyzes the syntax and Chinese usage of VCO structure and VOVC structure. On the whole, the frequency of VCO and VOVC structure in VC structure is relatively low. The longer the modifier before the object, the harder it is to enter the VCO structure. It is not easy to add modifiers to the front of the VOVC structure. The “C” in the VCO structure provides background information for subsequent clauses, indicating that this amount exceeds the preset, the “VO” in the VOVC structure represents an event, the VO provides background information, and whether the focus is reread on “C” is determined by rereading. VOVC structure is mainly used in spoken language, especially in some high-frequency words, and VCO structure is used in written language and spoken language. It is hoped that this paper can also provide some reference for the teaching of Chinese as a foreign language. 本文以前人的研究成果为基础,在掌握大量语料的前提下,对VCO结构和VOVC结构的差异进行了系统的分析。 从句法的角度讲,宾语前的修饰语越长越不易进入VCO结构。当动词前有修饰成分时,可以进入VCO结构,但不易进入VOVC结构。VCO结构前面不易出现修饰成分的原因是焦点在“C”上,不管“C”为大量还是小量,都表示这个量超出了预设,VCO结构是为后续小句提供背景信息。VOVC结构中“V”的前面不易加入修饰成分的原因是“VO”是表示一个事件,VOVC结构是由“VO+VC”构成,主要是陈述做某件事所经历的时间,VO提供的是背景信息,焦点是否在“C”上由是否重读来决定。 从语体上说,VOVC结构主要用于口语,特别适用于一部分高频词;VCO结构用于书面语和口语。本文的遗憾是没有从历时的角度进行考察,但从共时的角度来分析,VOVC结构和VCO结构在语体上使用的不同,以及在句法上和语用上差异值得我们做更深入的分析。 希望本文对时量补语在对外汉语的教学方面也能提供一定的参考。

      • KCI등재후보

        Korean Intonational Phrases Do Not Distinguish Dependent Clauses from Within-Clause Units

        Ogyoung Lee 서울대학교 외국어교육연구소 2018 외국어교육연구 Vol.22 No.-

        This study examined how adult native speakers of Korean prosodically structure their speech during spontaneous story telling. It asked participants to produce a spontaneous story from a picture book in L1 Korean. Participants’ phonological phrasing (via pause) was analyzed with reference to eight hierarchical syntactic structures defined in the study. The results suggested three-level hierarchy in phonological structure in spoken Korean: between sentences > between independent clauses > between a dependent clause and the matrix clause = any within-clause boundaries. Korean speakers paused the longest prior to starting a new sentence. They paused statistically significantly shorter between two independent clauses (e.g., coordinate clauses and adverbial clauses), and even shorter between a dependent clause (e.g., complement clauses and relative clauses) and the matrix clause. Interestingly, however, the last type of clausal boundaries, i.e., between interdependent but separate clauses, did not differ from any of the observed within-clause boundaries with respect to intonation structure marked by pauses. These results are consistent with the literature as to across-clause boundaries, but contra the traditional prediction that syntactic hierarchical structure may be mapped onto phonological structure: clausal boundaries are prosodically treated the same as within-clausal boundaries. Crosslinguistic and educational implications are discussed.

      • KCI등재후보

        Lexico-Syntactic Structures of Spatial Expressions

        Eugene Chung 고려대학교 언어정보연구소 2018 언어정보 Vol.26 No.-

        Eugene Chung. 2018. Lexico-Syntactic Structures of Spatial Expressions. Language Information, Volume 26. 79-102. This work aims to model a lexico-syntactic structure for various spatial configurations. In English, spatial expressions are similar to transitive verbs in the pattern where prepositions take arguments to describe spatial relationships. The meaning expressed by a spatial preposition indicates how its arguments physically relate to each other in space. The spatial preposition has two arguments, a Figure and a Ground. This study adapts Pustejovsky's (1991, 1995) argument structure and co-composition from the Generative Lexicon Theory in order to represent multiple configurations. Various features are employed to describe spatial relations and spatial entities. Arrangement features and physical relationship features are for spatial relations. Region features, dimensionality, and orientation features are utilized to provide fine-grained specification of spatial entities. The values for arrangements are CONTACT, ADJACENCY, OVERLAP, INCLUSION, SURROUNDING. Region feature takes one of SURFACE, BOUNDARY, TOP, BOTTOM, INTERIOR, EXTERIOR. For the dimensionality, we have 1DIM, 2DIM, 3DIM values. HORIZONTAL and VERTICAL are the values for the orientation feature. A locus structure is introduced to encode various spatial features that are related with words about spatial entities and spatial relations. A spatial preposition and its arguments construct spatial expressions. They compose phrasal level meanings through co-specification with the locus values in the loci structure. The proposed framework can be utilized to represent the formal rules of encoding spatial expressions.

      • KCI등재

        영어발화에 나타난 타동사 통사구조 유지성

        박분주 현대영어교육학회 2009 현대영어교육 Vol.10 No.1

        Syntactic persistence is defined as a phenomenon that the structure tends to be carried over to the next utterance. The purpose of this study is to investigate syntactic persistence in first and second language speakers of English. Forty university students participated and consisted of twenty English L1 speakers of English and twenty L2 speakers of English with Korean as their L1. The experimental paradigm involved repetition of an auditory stimulus, followed by picture description. The target structures were transitive alternate structures (active vs. passive). Overall, the effects of syntactic persistence were found in English transitive structure. The results showed that syntactic priming pattern was similar between English native speakers(L1) and second language speakers of English (L2). This study contributes to further universal accounts of syntactic persistence in language production theory. The pedagogical implication that this study might have is that syntactic structure might be learned inherently through exchanging utterances with native speaker of English.

      연관 검색어 추천

      이 검색어로 많이 본 자료

      활용도 높은 자료

      해외이동버튼