RISS 학술연구정보서비스

검색
다국어 입력

http://chineseinput.net/에서 pinyin(병음)방식으로 중국어를 변환할 수 있습니다.

변환된 중국어를 복사하여 사용하시면 됩니다.

예시)
  • 中文 을 입력하시려면 zhongwen을 입력하시고 space를누르시면됩니다.
  • 北京 을 입력하시려면 beijing을 입력하시고 space를 누르시면 됩니다.
닫기
    인기검색어 순위 펼치기

    RISS 인기검색어

      Effects of Scaffolding Type and Working Memory on Programming Performance and Computational Thinking Skills in Elementary School Students’ Programming Learning = 초등학생 프로그래밍 학습에서 스캐폴딩 유형과 작동기억이 프로그래밍 성취도 및 컴퓨팅 사고력에 미치는 영향

      한글로보기

      https://www.riss.kr/link?id=T16670725

      • 0

        상세조회
      • 0

        다운로드
      서지정보 열기
      • 내보내기
      • 내책장담기
      • 공유하기
      • 오류접수

      부가정보

      다국어 초록 (Multilingual Abstract)

      Various studies have documented that scaffolding is sociocultural theory-driven assistance for improving learning outcomes. Cognitive and metacognitive scaffolding, however, have received scant attention in terms of their comparative effects on learners' programming development. On the other hand, learning programming involves working memory. As a partial attempt in this respect, the current study aimed to explore how cognitive and metacognitive scaffolding, alone or in combination, influence elementary school students’ programming performance and computational thinking skills while considering their working memory. For this purpose, this study employed a quasi-experimental pretest-posttest design with 173 fifth graders as participants. Four intact classes were randomly assigned to a control group and three experimental groups: cognitive scaffolding, metacognitive scaffolding, and both forms of scaffolding, namely, cognitive-and-metacognitive scaffolding. Before learning to program in Scratch, participants were required to complete a prior programming knowledge test, working memory tests, and a computational thinking test. After a seven-week programming learning activity, every participant was expected to finish a final project that served as an assessment of programming skills. Subsequently, participants were given post-tests of programming knowledge and computational thinking. Furthermore, students were classified as having low and high levels of working memory with regard to central executive working memory, phonological working memory, and visuo-spatial working memory.
      Data from a total of 168 students were analyzed due to some absences. Two-way MANCOVAs and ANCOVAs were implemented to investigate the two research questions. The following are the major findings of the study.
      First, scaffolding had a significantly superior effect on programming knowledge but not programming skills compared to non-scaffolding instruction. The cognitive-and-metacognitive scaffolding group did not show significantly better scores in programming knowledge and skills than the cognitive or metacognitive scaffolding group. Likewise, no significant difference was found between the metacognitive and cognitive scaffolding groups in programming knowledge and skills.
      Second, there was a significant difference between the experimental and control groups in computational thinking skills. However, students in the cognitive-and-metacognitive scaffolding group did not develop significantly greater computational thinking skills than those in the cognitive or metacognitive scaffolding group. Similarly, the metacognitive scaffolding group did not show significantly higher computational thinking skills than the cognitive scaffolding group.
      Third, the findings indicated that students with high capacities of working memory (central executive, phonological, and visual-spatial working memory) scored significantly higher on programming knowledge, programming skills, and computational thinking skills than low-capacity working memory students.
      Fourth, the interaction effects were only found between scaffolding type and central executive working memory on programming knowledge. For the students with low central executive working memory, there was a significant difference between the experimental and control groups. As for the students with high central executive working memory, the experimental groups significantly outperformed the control group. Moreover, high-capacity working memory students performed significantly better in the cognitive-and-metacognitive scaffolding group than in the cognitive or metacognitive scaffolding group. In addition, high-capacity working memory students also obtained significantly higher scores in the metacognitive scaffolding group than in the cognitive scaffolding group.
      This study provides implications for the design and development of scaffolding based on Vygotsky’s theory of cognitive and metacognitive mediation. In addition, more attention should be paid to individual differences in working memory during programming activities since working memory is greatly important for students’ success in programming performance and computational thinking skills.
      번역하기

      Various studies have documented that scaffolding is sociocultural theory-driven assistance for improving learning outcomes. Cognitive and metacognitive scaffolding, however, have received scant attention in terms of their comparative effects on learne...

      Various studies have documented that scaffolding is sociocultural theory-driven assistance for improving learning outcomes. Cognitive and metacognitive scaffolding, however, have received scant attention in terms of their comparative effects on learners' programming development. On the other hand, learning programming involves working memory. As a partial attempt in this respect, the current study aimed to explore how cognitive and metacognitive scaffolding, alone or in combination, influence elementary school students’ programming performance and computational thinking skills while considering their working memory. For this purpose, this study employed a quasi-experimental pretest-posttest design with 173 fifth graders as participants. Four intact classes were randomly assigned to a control group and three experimental groups: cognitive scaffolding, metacognitive scaffolding, and both forms of scaffolding, namely, cognitive-and-metacognitive scaffolding. Before learning to program in Scratch, participants were required to complete a prior programming knowledge test, working memory tests, and a computational thinking test. After a seven-week programming learning activity, every participant was expected to finish a final project that served as an assessment of programming skills. Subsequently, participants were given post-tests of programming knowledge and computational thinking. Furthermore, students were classified as having low and high levels of working memory with regard to central executive working memory, phonological working memory, and visuo-spatial working memory.
      Data from a total of 168 students were analyzed due to some absences. Two-way MANCOVAs and ANCOVAs were implemented to investigate the two research questions. The following are the major findings of the study.
      First, scaffolding had a significantly superior effect on programming knowledge but not programming skills compared to non-scaffolding instruction. The cognitive-and-metacognitive scaffolding group did not show significantly better scores in programming knowledge and skills than the cognitive or metacognitive scaffolding group. Likewise, no significant difference was found between the metacognitive and cognitive scaffolding groups in programming knowledge and skills.
      Second, there was a significant difference between the experimental and control groups in computational thinking skills. However, students in the cognitive-and-metacognitive scaffolding group did not develop significantly greater computational thinking skills than those in the cognitive or metacognitive scaffolding group. Similarly, the metacognitive scaffolding group did not show significantly higher computational thinking skills than the cognitive scaffolding group.
      Third, the findings indicated that students with high capacities of working memory (central executive, phonological, and visual-spatial working memory) scored significantly higher on programming knowledge, programming skills, and computational thinking skills than low-capacity working memory students.
      Fourth, the interaction effects were only found between scaffolding type and central executive working memory on programming knowledge. For the students with low central executive working memory, there was a significant difference between the experimental and control groups. As for the students with high central executive working memory, the experimental groups significantly outperformed the control group. Moreover, high-capacity working memory students performed significantly better in the cognitive-and-metacognitive scaffolding group than in the cognitive or metacognitive scaffolding group. In addition, high-capacity working memory students also obtained significantly higher scores in the metacognitive scaffolding group than in the cognitive scaffolding group.
      This study provides implications for the design and development of scaffolding based on Vygotsky’s theory of cognitive and metacognitive mediation. In addition, more attention should be paid to individual differences in working memory during programming activities since working memory is greatly important for students’ success in programming performance and computational thinking skills.

      더보기

      국문 초록 (Abstract)

      그동안 많은 연구에서 학습성과 향상을 위한 사회문화이론의 보조 도구로의 스캐폴딩이 자주 언급되어 왔다. 그러나 학습자의 프로그래밍 성취를 위한 다양한 인지적 및 메타인지적 스캐폴딩을 비교한 연구는 부족한 실정이다. 한편, 작동기억은 프로그래밍 학습에서 중요한 역할을 하는 것으로 나타났다. 이와 관련하여 본 연구는 초등학생들의 작동기억을 고려하여 인지적 스캐폴딩과 메타인지적 스캐폴딩이 학생의 프로그래밍 성취도 및 컴퓨팅 사고력에 어떤 영향을 미치는지를 검증하였다. 연구대상은 중국 5 학년 학생 173 명이었다. 이 연구에서는 이질통제집단 사전-사후검사 설계를 활용하였다. 즉, 연구의 실험집단은 인지적 스캐폴딩, 메타인지적 스캐폴딩, 인지적 및 메타인지적 스캐폴딩 집단으로 구성되었고, 통제집단은 스캐폴딩을 제공받지 않았다. Scratch 프로그래밍 활동에 앞서 참가자들에게 사전 프로그래밍 지식, 작동기억, 컴퓨팅 사고력 등의 검사를 수행하도록 요청하였다. 7 주간의 프로그래밍 활동 후, 참가자들은 프로그래밍 기능의 평가로서 최종 프로젝트를 완성하였다. 이어 프로그래밍 지식 사후 검사, 컴퓨팅 사고력 검사를 실시하였다. 또한 학생들의 작동기억은 중앙집행 작동기억, 언어적 작동기억, 시공간적 작동기억 상에서 높은 집단과 낮은 집단으로 분류되었다.
      173 명의 학생 중 최종 168 명의 학생들의 자료가 취합되었으며, 이를 분석하였다. 두 가지 연구 문제를 해결하기 위하여 이원 다변량공분산분석(MANCOVA)과 공분산분석(ANCOVA)를 실시하였다. 연구 결과는 다음과 같다.
      첫째, 스캐폴딩을 제공받은 집단은 스캐폴딩을 제공받지 않은 집단보다 프로그래밍 지식이 유의하게 높은 것으로 나타났다. 그러나 실험집단과 통제집단간 프로그램 기능에는 유의한 차이가 없었다. 인지적 및 메타인지적 스캐폴딩을 동시 제공받은 집단이 인지적 스캐폴딩 집단이나 메타인지적 스캐폴딩 집단에 비해 프로그래밍 지식과 기능 상에서 유의하게 더 높은 점수를 보이지 않았다. 또한, 메타인지적 스캐폴딩 집단과 인지적 스캐폴딩 집단은 프로그래밍 지식과 기능에서도 유의한 차이가 없었다.
      둘째, 컴퓨팅 사고력에는 실험집단과 통제집단 사이에 유의한 차이가 있었다. 그러나 인지적 및 메타인지적 스캐폴딩을 동시 제공받은 집단은 인지적 스캐폴딩 집단이나 메타인지적 스캐폴딩 집단보다 유의하게 높은 컴퓨팅 사고력을 보이지는 못 하였다. 또한 메타인지적 스캐폴딩 집단과 인지적 스캐폴딩 집단 사이에 컴퓨팅 사고력에는 유의한 차이가 없었다.
      셋째, 작동기억이 높은 학생들은 작동기억이 낮은 학생들보다 프로그래밍 지식, 프로그래밍 기능, 컴퓨팅 사고력이 유의하게 높게 나타났다.
      넷째, 프로그래밍 지식에 있어서 스캐폴딩 유형과 중앙집행 작동기억의 상호작용 효과가 있었다. 중앙집행 작동기억이 낮은 학생들의 경우 실험집단과 통제 집단 간에만 유의한 차이가 있었고, 중앙집행 작동기억이 높은 학생들의 경우도 동일한 결과를 보였다. 또한 중앙집행 작동기억이 높은 학생들은 인지적 및 메타인지적 스캐폴딩을 동시 제공받은 집단에서 인지적 스캐폴딩 집단이나 메타인지적 스캐폴딩 집단에서보다 프로그래밍 지식 점수가 유의하게 더 높았다. 또한 중앙집행 작동기억이 높은 학생들은 메타인지적 스캐폴딩 집단에서 인지적 스캐폴딩 집단에서보다 프로그래밍 지식 점수가 유의하게 더 높았다.
      본 연구는 초등학생의 프로그래밍 학습을 위한 Vygotsky 의 인지적 및 메타인지적 중개이론에 입각한 스캐폴딩 설계 및 개발에 시사하는 바가 크다. 또한 작동기억은 학생들의 프로그래밍 성과와 컴퓨팅 사고력에서 중요한 요소 중 하나이므로 프로그래밍 활동 중 작동기억의 개인차에 더욱 유의할 필요가 있다.
      번역하기

      그동안 많은 연구에서 학습성과 향상을 위한 사회문화이론의 보조 도구로의 스캐폴딩이 자주 언급되어 왔다. 그러나 학습자의 프로그래밍 성취를 위한 다양한 인지적 및 메타인지적 스캐폴...

      그동안 많은 연구에서 학습성과 향상을 위한 사회문화이론의 보조 도구로의 스캐폴딩이 자주 언급되어 왔다. 그러나 학습자의 프로그래밍 성취를 위한 다양한 인지적 및 메타인지적 스캐폴딩을 비교한 연구는 부족한 실정이다. 한편, 작동기억은 프로그래밍 학습에서 중요한 역할을 하는 것으로 나타났다. 이와 관련하여 본 연구는 초등학생들의 작동기억을 고려하여 인지적 스캐폴딩과 메타인지적 스캐폴딩이 학생의 프로그래밍 성취도 및 컴퓨팅 사고력에 어떤 영향을 미치는지를 검증하였다. 연구대상은 중국 5 학년 학생 173 명이었다. 이 연구에서는 이질통제집단 사전-사후검사 설계를 활용하였다. 즉, 연구의 실험집단은 인지적 스캐폴딩, 메타인지적 스캐폴딩, 인지적 및 메타인지적 스캐폴딩 집단으로 구성되었고, 통제집단은 스캐폴딩을 제공받지 않았다. Scratch 프로그래밍 활동에 앞서 참가자들에게 사전 프로그래밍 지식, 작동기억, 컴퓨팅 사고력 등의 검사를 수행하도록 요청하였다. 7 주간의 프로그래밍 활동 후, 참가자들은 프로그래밍 기능의 평가로서 최종 프로젝트를 완성하였다. 이어 프로그래밍 지식 사후 검사, 컴퓨팅 사고력 검사를 실시하였다. 또한 학생들의 작동기억은 중앙집행 작동기억, 언어적 작동기억, 시공간적 작동기억 상에서 높은 집단과 낮은 집단으로 분류되었다.
      173 명의 학생 중 최종 168 명의 학생들의 자료가 취합되었으며, 이를 분석하였다. 두 가지 연구 문제를 해결하기 위하여 이원 다변량공분산분석(MANCOVA)과 공분산분석(ANCOVA)를 실시하였다. 연구 결과는 다음과 같다.
      첫째, 스캐폴딩을 제공받은 집단은 스캐폴딩을 제공받지 않은 집단보다 프로그래밍 지식이 유의하게 높은 것으로 나타났다. 그러나 실험집단과 통제집단간 프로그램 기능에는 유의한 차이가 없었다. 인지적 및 메타인지적 스캐폴딩을 동시 제공받은 집단이 인지적 스캐폴딩 집단이나 메타인지적 스캐폴딩 집단에 비해 프로그래밍 지식과 기능 상에서 유의하게 더 높은 점수를 보이지 않았다. 또한, 메타인지적 스캐폴딩 집단과 인지적 스캐폴딩 집단은 프로그래밍 지식과 기능에서도 유의한 차이가 없었다.
      둘째, 컴퓨팅 사고력에는 실험집단과 통제집단 사이에 유의한 차이가 있었다. 그러나 인지적 및 메타인지적 스캐폴딩을 동시 제공받은 집단은 인지적 스캐폴딩 집단이나 메타인지적 스캐폴딩 집단보다 유의하게 높은 컴퓨팅 사고력을 보이지는 못 하였다. 또한 메타인지적 스캐폴딩 집단과 인지적 스캐폴딩 집단 사이에 컴퓨팅 사고력에는 유의한 차이가 없었다.
      셋째, 작동기억이 높은 학생들은 작동기억이 낮은 학생들보다 프로그래밍 지식, 프로그래밍 기능, 컴퓨팅 사고력이 유의하게 높게 나타났다.
      넷째, 프로그래밍 지식에 있어서 스캐폴딩 유형과 중앙집행 작동기억의 상호작용 효과가 있었다. 중앙집행 작동기억이 낮은 학생들의 경우 실험집단과 통제 집단 간에만 유의한 차이가 있었고, 중앙집행 작동기억이 높은 학생들의 경우도 동일한 결과를 보였다. 또한 중앙집행 작동기억이 높은 학생들은 인지적 및 메타인지적 스캐폴딩을 동시 제공받은 집단에서 인지적 스캐폴딩 집단이나 메타인지적 스캐폴딩 집단에서보다 프로그래밍 지식 점수가 유의하게 더 높았다. 또한 중앙집행 작동기억이 높은 학생들은 메타인지적 스캐폴딩 집단에서 인지적 스캐폴딩 집단에서보다 프로그래밍 지식 점수가 유의하게 더 높았다.
      본 연구는 초등학생의 프로그래밍 학습을 위한 Vygotsky 의 인지적 및 메타인지적 중개이론에 입각한 스캐폴딩 설계 및 개발에 시사하는 바가 크다. 또한 작동기억은 학생들의 프로그래밍 성과와 컴퓨팅 사고력에서 중요한 요소 중 하나이므로 프로그래밍 활동 중 작동기억의 개인차에 더욱 유의할 필요가 있다.

      더보기

      목차 (Table of Contents)

      • Contents ⅰ
      • List of Tables ⅲ
      • List of Figures ⅴ
      • List of Abbreviations ⅵ
      • (Abstract)ⅶ
      • Contents ⅰ
      • List of Tables ⅲ
      • List of Figures ⅴ
      • List of Abbreviations ⅵ
      • (Abstract)ⅶ
      • I. Introduction 1
      • 1. Necessity and Purpose 1
      • 2. Research Questions 6
      • 3. Definition of Terms 6
      • Ⅱ. Literature Review 10
      • 1. Programming Education in K-12 Contexts 10
      • 2. Scaffolding and Working Memory 14
      • 3. Scaffolding, Working Memory and Programming Performance 26
      • 4. Scaffolding, Working Memory and Computational Thinking Skills 33
      • 5. Interaction Effects of Scaffolding Type and Working Memory 41
      • Ⅲ. Research Hypotheses 45
      • Ⅳ. Method 51
      • 1. Participants 51
      • 2. Instruments 52
      • 3. Learning Tasks and Scaffolding Design 59
      • 4. Experimental Design 65
      • 5. Experimental Procedure 66
      • 6. Data Analysis 68
      • Ⅴ. Results 69
      • 1. Effects of Scaffolding Type and Working Memory on Programming Performance 70
      • 2. Effects of Scaffolding Type and Working Memory on Computational Thinking Skills 80
      • Ⅵ. Discussion and Conclusion 88
      • 1. Discussion 88
      • 2. Conclusion 95
      • References 98
      • (국문 초록) 128
      • Appendix 131
      더보기

      참고문헌 (Reference) 논문관계도

      1 Bruner, J. S., Wood, D., Ross, G., "The role of tutoring in problem solving", 17(2), 89–100. https://doi. org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1976. tb00381. x, 1976

      2 Baddeley, A, "Working memory and language: An overview", 36(3), 189–208. https://doi. org/10.1016/S0021-9924(03)00019-4, 2003

      3 Engle, R. W., Turner, M. L., "Is working memory capacity task dependent?", 28(2), 127–154. https://doi. org/10.1016/0749-596X(89)90040-5, 1989

      4 Shute, V. J., "Who is likely to acquire programming skills?", 7(1), 1–24. https://doi. org/10.2190/VQJD-T1YD-5WVB-RYPJ, 1991

      5 Carpenter, P. A, Daneman, M., "Individual differences in working memory and reading", 19(4), 450–466. https://doi. org/10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90312-6, 1980

      6 Engle, R. W., "Role of working memory capacity in cognitive control", 51(1), 517–526. https://doi. org/10.1086/650572, 2010

      7 Haywood, H. C., Karpov, Y. V., "Two ways to elaborate Vygotsky's concept of mediation", 53(1), 27–36. https://doi. org/10.1037/0003-066X.53.1.27, 1998

      8 Cohen , J, "Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences", 2nd ed., 1988

      9 Das-Smaal, E., de Jong, P., "The star counting test: An attention test for children", 11(6), 597–604. https://doi. org/10.1016/0191-8869(90)90043-Q, 1990

      10 Robins, A., Rountree, N., Rountree, J., "Learning and teaching programming: A review and discussion", 13(2), 137–172. https://doi. org/10.1076/csed.13.2.137.14200, 2003

      1 Bruner, J. S., Wood, D., Ross, G., "The role of tutoring in problem solving", 17(2), 89–100. https://doi. org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1976. tb00381. x, 1976

      2 Baddeley, A, "Working memory and language: An overview", 36(3), 189–208. https://doi. org/10.1016/S0021-9924(03)00019-4, 2003

      3 Engle, R. W., Turner, M. L., "Is working memory capacity task dependent?", 28(2), 127–154. https://doi. org/10.1016/0749-596X(89)90040-5, 1989

      4 Shute, V. J., "Who is likely to acquire programming skills?", 7(1), 1–24. https://doi. org/10.2190/VQJD-T1YD-5WVB-RYPJ, 1991

      5 Carpenter, P. A, Daneman, M., "Individual differences in working memory and reading", 19(4), 450–466. https://doi. org/10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90312-6, 1980

      6 Engle, R. W., "Role of working memory capacity in cognitive control", 51(1), 517–526. https://doi. org/10.1086/650572, 2010

      7 Haywood, H. C., Karpov, Y. V., "Two ways to elaborate Vygotsky's concept of mediation", 53(1), 27–36. https://doi. org/10.1037/0003-066X.53.1.27, 1998

      8 Cohen , J, "Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences", 2nd ed., 1988

      9 Das-Smaal, E., de Jong, P., "The star counting test: An attention test for children", 11(6), 597–604. https://doi. org/10.1016/0191-8869(90)90043-Q, 1990

      10 Robins, A., Rountree, N., Rountree, J., "Learning and teaching programming: A review and discussion", 13(2), 137–172. https://doi. org/10.1076/csed.13.2.137.14200, 2003

      11 Das-Smaal, E. A., de Jong, P. F., Koopmans, J. R., "Working memory, attentional regulation and the Star Counting Test", 14(6), 815–824. https://doi. org/10.1016/0191-8869(93)90094-J, 1993

      12 Molenaar, I., van Boxtel, C. A., Sleegers, P. J., "The effects of scaffolding metacognitive activities in small groups", 26(6), 1727–1738. https://doi. org/10.1016/j. chb.2010.06.022, 2010

      13 Hargrove, R. A., Nietfeld, J. L., "The impact of metacognitive instruction on creative problem solving", 83(3), 291–318. https://doi. org/10.1080/00220973.2013.876604, 2015

      14 Bunting, M. F., Wilhelm, O., Kane, M. J., Hambrick, D. Z., Engle, R. W., Conway, A. R. A., "Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user's guide", 12(5), 769–786. https://doi. org/10.3758/BF03196772, 2005

      15 Kalelioğlu, F., "A new way of teaching programming skills to K-12 students: Code. org", Computers 52, 200–210. https://doi. org/10.1016/j. chb.2015.05.047, 2015

      16 Chiu, M. M., van Boxtel, C., Sleegers, P., Molenaar, I., "Scaffolding of small groups’ metacognitive activities with an avatar", 6(4), 601–624. https://doi. org/10.1007/s11412-011-9130-z, 2011

      17 Weigend, M., "The digital woodlouse–Scaffolding in science-related scratch projects", 13(2), 293–305. https://doi. org/10.15388/infedu.2014.18, 2014

      18 Booth, J., Toye, M., Robertson, J., Gray, S., "The relationship between executive functions and computational thinking", 3(4), 35–49. https://doi. org/10.21585/ijcses. v3i4.76, 2020

      19 Baddeley, A., Leigh, E., Jarrold, C., Gunn, D., Bayliss, D., "Mapping the developmental constraints on working memory span performance", 41, 579–597. https://doi. org/10.1037/0012-1649.41.4.579, 2005

      20 Cowan, N., "The magical mystery four: How is working memory capacity limited, and why?", 19(1), 51–57. https://doi. org/10.1177/0963721409359277, 2010

      21 Bagheri, M. S., Yamini, M., Razaghi, M., "The impact of cognitive scaffolding on Iranian EFL learners' speaking skill", 12(4), 95–112, 2019

      22 Lecerf, T., Roulin, J. L., "Distinction between visuo-spatial short-term-memory and working memory span tasks", 65(1), 37–54. https://doi. org/10.1024/1421-0185.65.1.37, 2006

      23 In M. J. Jacobson, Soloway, E., R. B. Kozma (Eds, Metcalf, S. J., Krajcik, J., "Model-It: A design retrospective Innovations in science and mathematics education", 1st ed., 77–115, 2000

      24 Adams, C., Lu, C., Cutumisu, M., "A scoping review of empirical research on recent computational thinking assessments", 28(6), 651–676. https://doi. org/10.1007/s10956-019-09799-3, 2019

      25 In J. V. Wertsch (Ed., Vygotsky, L. S., "The genesis of higher mental functions The concept of activity in Soviet psychology", pp. 144–188, 1981

      26 Chen, J., Zhong, B., Wang, Q., Li, Y., "An exploration of three-dimensional integrated assessment for computational thinking", 53(4), 562–590. https://doi. org/10.1177/0735633115608444, 2016

      27 Daley, B. J., Roessger, K. M., Hafez, D. A., "Effects of teaching concept mapping using practice, feedback, and relational framing", 54, 11– 21. https://doi. org/10.1016/j. learninstruc.2018.01.011, 2018

      28 Moreno-León, J., Román-González, M., Robles, G., Pérez-González, J. C., "Extending the nomological network of computational thinking with non-cognitive factors", 80, 441–459. https://doi. org/10.1016/j. chb.2017.09.030, 2018

      29 Flavell, J. H., "Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive– developmental inquiry", 34(10), 906–911. https://doi. org/10.1037/0003-066X.34.10.906, 1979

      30 Mayer, R., Shneiderman, B., "Syntactic/semantic interactions in programmer behavior: A model and experimental results", 8(3), 219–238. https://doi. org/10.1007/BF00977789, 1979

      31 Hoffman, B., Schraw, G., "The influence of self-efficacy and working memory capacity on problem-solving efficiency", 19(1), 91–100. https://doi. org/10.1016/j. lindif.2008.08.001, 2009

      32 Jacobs, J. E., Paris, S. G., "Children's metacognition about reading: Issues in definition, measurement, and instruction", 22(3-4), 255– 278. https://doi. org/10.1080/00461520.1987.9653052, 1987

      33 Darhmaoui, H., Ouahbi, I., Lahmine, S., Kaddari, F., Elachqar, A., "Learning basic programming concepts by creating games with scratch programming environment", 191, 1479–1482. https://doi. org/10.1016/j. sbspro.2015.04.224, 2015

      34 Magimairaj, B. M., O’Malley, M. H., Montgomery, J. W., "Role of working memory in typically developing children’s complex sentence comprehension", 37, 331–354. https://doi. org/10.1007/s10936-008-9077-z, 2008

      35 Mackiewicz, J., Thompson, I, "Instruction, cognitive scaffolding, and motivational scaffolding in writing center tutoring", 42(1), 54–78. https://www. jstor. org/stable/compstud.42.1.0054, 2014

      36 Hagge, J., "Scratching beyond the surface of literacy: Programming for early adolescent gifted students", 40(3), 154–162. https://doi. org/10.1177/1076217517707233, 2017

      37 Moreno-León, J., Román-González, M., Robles, G., Pérez-González, J. C., "Can computational talent be detected? Predictive validity of the Computational Thinking Test", 18, 47–58. https://doi. org/10.1016/j. ijcci.2018.06.004, 2018

      38 Azevedo, R., Winters, F. I., Moos, D. C., Greene, J. A., Cromley, J. G., "Adaptive human scaffolding facilitates adolescents’ self-regulated learning with hypermedia", 33(5), 381–412. https://doi. org/10.1007/s11251-005-1273-8, 2005

      39 Passolunghi, M. C., Siegel, L., "Working memory and access to numerical information in children with disability in mathematics", 88, 348–367. https://doi. org/10.1016/j. jecp.2004.04.002, 2004

      40 Casallas, R., Reyes, A., Restrepo, S., Hernández, M., Flórez, F. B., Danies, G., "Changing a generation’s way of thinking: Teaching computational thinking through programming", 87(4), 834–860. https://doi. org/10.3102/0034654317710096, 2017

      41 Kim, H. S., Yum, S. C., Schallert, D. L., "Effects of prior knowledge, working memory, and navigation tools on performance with hypertext", 18(1), 79–108, 2002

      42 Kuo, C. H., Prat, C. S., Mottarella, M. J., Madhyastha, T. M., "Relating natural language aptitude to individual differences in learning programming languages", 10(1), 1–10. https://doi. org/10.1038/s41598-020-60661-8, 2020

      43 Shao, M., Zhao, L., Su, Y. S., "Effect of mind mapping on creative thinking of children in scratch visual programming education", 60(4), 906–929. https://doi. org/10.1177/07356331211053383, 2022

      44 Ghisletta, P., Lecerf, T., Jouffray, C., "Intraindividual variability and level of performance in four visuo-spatial working memory tasks", 63(4), 261–272. https://doi. org/10.1024/1421-0185.63.4.261, 2004

      45 Aleven, V., Roll, I., McLaren, B. M., Koedinger, K. R., "Designing for metacognition—applying cognitive tutor principles to the tutoring of help seeking", 2(2), 125–140. https://doi. org/10.1007/s11409-007-9010-0, 2007

      46 Kliegl, R., Mayr, U., "Sequential and coordinative complexity: age-based processing limitations in figural transformations", 19(6), 1297–1320. https://doi. org/10.1037/0278-7393.19.6.1297, 1993

      47 Azevedo, R., Seibert, D., Cromley, J. G., "Does adaptive scaffolding facilitate students’ ability to regulate their learning with hypermedia?", 29(3), 344–370. https://doi. org/10.1016/j. cedpsych.2003.09.002, 2004

      48 Koh, J. H. L., Lye, S. Y., "Review on teaching and learning of computational thinking through programming: What is next for K-12?", Computers 41, 51–61. https://doi. org/10.1016/j. chb.2014.09.012, 2014

      49 Barth-Cohen, L., Shen, J., Jiang, S., Huang, X., Eltoukhy, M., Chen, G., "Assessing elementary students’ computational thinking in everyday reasoning and robotics programming", 109, 162–175. https://doi. org/10.1016/j. compedu.2017.03.001, 2017

      50 Bergin, D., Teo, T., Lee, C. B., "Children’s use of metacognition in solving everyday problems: An initial study from an Asian context", 36(3), 89–102. https://doi. org/10.1007/BF03216907, 2009

      51 Passolunghi, M. C., Vercelloni, B., Schadee, H., "The precursors of mathematics learning: Working memory, phonological ability, and numerical competence", 22, 165–184. https://doi. org/10.1016/j. cogdev.2006.09.001, 2007

      52 Kirschner, P. A., Wu, B., Wang, M., Spector, J. M., "Using cognitive mapping to foster deeper learning with complex problems in a computer-based environment", Computers 87, 450–458. https://doi. org/10.1016/j. chb.2018.01.024, 2018

      53 Curran, P. J., West, S. G., Finch, J. F., "The robustness of test statistics to nonnormality and specification error in confirmatory factor analysis", 1(1), 16–29. https://doi. org/10.1037/1082-989x.1.1.16, 1996

      54 Ahmadi Safa, M., Motaghi, F., "Cognitive vs. metacognitive scaffolding strategies and EFL learners’ listening comprehension development", 1–24. https://doi. org/10.1177/13621688211021821, 2021

      55 Lee, J., Noh, J., "Effects of robotics programming on the computational thinking and creativity of elementary school students", 68(1), 463–484. https://doi. org/10.1007/s11423-019-09708-w, 2020

      56 Falloon, G., "An analysis of young students’ thinking when completing basic coding tasks using Scratch Jnr. On the iPad", 32(6), 576–593. https://doi. org/10.1111/jcal.12155, 2016

      57 Oberauer, K., Wilhelm, O., Süß, H. M., Schulze, R., "Working memory and intelligence-Their correlation and their relation: Comment on Ackerman, Beier, and Boyle", 131, 61–65. https://doi. org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.61, 2005

      58 Kim, H. S., Kim. J. Y., "The effects of navigation methods and working memory on learning achievement and cognitive load in hypertext", 19(1), 109–128, 2013

      59 Chen, M. P., Feng, C. Y., "The effects of goal specificity and scaffolding on programming performance and self‐regulation in game design", 45(2), 285–302. https://doi. org/10.1111/bjet.12022, 2014

      60 Jiménez-Fernández, C., Román-González, M., Pérez-González, J. C., "Which cognitive abilities underlie computational thinking? Criterion validity of the Computational Thinking Test", 72, 678–691. https://doi. org/10.1016/j. chb.2016.08.047, 2017

      61 Aksit, O., Wiebe, E. N., "Exploring force and motion concepts in middle grades using computational modeling: A classroom intervention study", 29(1), 65–82. https://doi. org/10.1007/s10956-019-09800-z, 2020

      62 Korkmaz, Ö., Bai, X., "Adapting computational thinking scale (CTS) for Chinese high school students and their thinking scale skills level", 6(1), 10–26. https://doi. org/10.17275/per.19.2.6.1, 2019

      63 Bocian, K., Zheng, X., Swanson, H. L., Moran, A. S., Lussier, C., "Generative strategies, working memory, and word problem solving accuracy in children at risk for math disabilities", 36(4), 203–214. https://doi. org/10.1177/0731948712464034, 2013

      64 In H. J. Hartman (Ed, Schraw, G., "Promoting general metacognitive awareness Metacognition in learning and instruction: Theory, research and practice", pp. 3– 16 https://doi. org/10.1007/978-94-017-2243-8_1, 2001

      65 Gülbahar, Y., Kalelioğlu, F., "The effects of teaching programming via scratch on problem solving skills: A discussion from learners' perspective", 13(1), 33–50, 2014

      66 Swanson, H. L., "Cognitive strategy interventions improve word problem solving and working memory in children with math disabilities", 6, Article 1099. https://doi. org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01099, 2015

      67 Hwang, G.-H., Yang, T.-C., Yang, S. J., Hwang, G.-J., "A two-tier test-based approach to improving students' computer-programming skills in a web-based learning environment", 18(1), 198– 210. https://www. jstor. org/stable/10.2307/jeductechsoci.18.1.198, 2015

      68 Durak, H. Y., Saritepeci, M., "Analysis of the relation between computational thinking skills and various variables with the structural equation model", 116, 191–202. https://doi. org/10.1016/j. compedu.2017.09.004, 2018

      69 Dagoc, D., Tan, D. A., "Effects of metacognitive scaffolding on the mathematics performance of grade 6 pupils in a cooperative learning environment", 7(4), 378–391, 2018

      70 Ardestani, E. M., Pitenoee, M. R., Modaberi, A., "The effect of cognitive and metacognitive writing strategies on content of the Iranian intermediate EFL learners’ writing", 8(3), 594–600. http://dx. doi. org/10.17507/jltr.0803.19, 2017

      71 Erol, O., Kurt, A. A., "The effects of teaching programming with scratch on pre-service information technology teachers' motivation and achievement", 77, 11–18. https://doi. org/10.1016/j. chb.2017.08.017, 2017

      72 Swanson, H. L., "Does cognitive strategy training on word problems compensate for working memory capacity in children with math difficulties?", 106, 831–848. https://doi. org/10.1037/a0035838, 2014

      73 Bergersen, G. R., Gustafsson, J. E., "Programming skill, knowledge, and working memory among professional software developers from an investment theory perspective", 32(4), 201–209. https://doi. org/10.1027/1614-0001/a000052, 2011

      74 Huang, I., Wang, H. Y., Hwang, G. J., "Comparison of the effects of project-based computer programming activities between mathematics-gifted students and average students", 3(1), 33–45. https://doi. org/10.1007/s40692-015-0047-9, 2016

      75 Carretti, B., Tencati, C., Drusi, S., Cornoldi, C., "Improving problem solving in primary school students: The effect of a training programme focusing on metacognition and working memory", 85(3), 424–439. https://doi. org/10.1111/bjep.12083, 2015

      76 Hu, L., Zhou, D., Sun, L., "Which way of design programming activities is more effective to promote K‐12 students' computational thinking skills? A metaanalysis", 37(4), 1048–1062. https://doi. org/10.1111/jcal.12545, 2021

      77 Bacelo, A., Pérez-Marín, D., Pizarro, C., Hijón-Neira, R., "Can computational thinking be improved by using a methodology based on metaphors and scratch to teach computer programming to children?", 105, Article 105849. https://doi. org/10.1016/j. chb.2018.12.027, 2020

      78 Román-González, M., Vázquez-Cano, E., Sáez-López, J. M., "Visual programming languages integrated across the curriculum in elementary school: A two year case study using Scratch in five schools", 97, 129–141. https://doi. org/10.1016/j. compedu.2016.03.003, 2016

      79 An, Y. J., Cao, L., "Examining the effects of metacognitive scaffolding on students’ design problem solving and metacognitive skills in an online environment", 10(4), 552–568. https://jolt. merlot. org/vol10no4/An_1214. pdf, 2014

      80 Bulu, S. T., Pedersen, S., "Scaffolding middle school students’ content knowledge and ill-structured problem solving in a problem-based hypermedia learning environment", 58(5), 507–529. https://doi. org/10.1007/s11423-010-9150-9, 2010

      81 Huang, C. S., Yang, S. J., Tern, M. Y., Su, A. Y., Hwang, W. Y., "Investigating the role of computer‐supported annotation in problem-solving-based teaching: An empirical study of a Scratch programming pedagogy", 45(4), 647–665. https://doi. org/10.1111/bjet.12058, 2014

      82 Hwang, G.-J., Wang, X.-M., Wang, H.-Y., Liang, Z.-Y., "Enhancing students’ computer programming performances, critical thinking awareness and attitudes towards programming: An online peer-assessment attempt", 20(4), 58–68. https://www. jstor. org/stable/26229205, 2017

      83 KaLyuga, S., Zhang, L., Lei, C., Lee, C., "Effectiveness of collaborative learning of computer programming under different learning group formations according to students' prior knowledge: A cognitive load perspective", 27(2), 171–192. https://www. learntechlib. org/primary/p/111825/, 2016

      84 Basnet, R. B., Saxena, A., Lemay, D. J., Doleck, T., Bazelais, P., "Algorithmic thinking, cooperativity, creativity, critical thinking, and problem solving: exploring the relationship between computational thinking skills and academic performance", 4(4), 355–369. https://doi. org/10.1007/s40692-017-0090-9, 2017

      85 Abassi, A. M., Nordin, Z. S., Channa, M. A., "Metacognitive scaffolding in reading comprehension: Classroom observations reveal strategies to overcome reading obstacles of engineering students at QUEST, Nawabshah, Sindh, Pakistan", 8(3), 131–140. http://doi. org/10.5539/ijel. v8n3p131, 2018

      86 Durak, H. Y., "Digital story design activities used for teaching programming effect on learning of programming concepts, programming self‐efficacy, and participation and analysis of student experiences", 34(6), 740–752. https://doi. org/10.1111/jcal.12281, 2018

      더보기

      분석정보

      View

      상세정보조회

      0

      Usage

      원문다운로드

      0

      대출신청

      0

      복사신청

      0

      EDDS신청

      0

      동일 주제 내 활용도 TOP

      더보기

      주제

      연도별 연구동향

      연도별 활용동향

      연관논문

      연구자 네트워크맵

      공동연구자 (7)

      유사연구자 (20) 활용도상위20명

      이 자료와 함께 이용한 RISS 자료

      나만을 위한 추천자료

      해외이동버튼